The Western image of Buddhism is that of a meditating monk. There’s something wrong with that image, but there’s something right about it as well. While meditation didn’t play a major role in traditional Buddhism until fairly recently (more about that below), it also appears undeniable that meditation plays a central role in the (historical) Buddha’s teaching. There are, however, many misunderstandings about the nature(s), role(s), and purpose(s) of meditation(s) in the Buddhist tradition. Others have attempted to correct some of those misunderstandings before – the chapter “A Primer on Buddhist Meditation” in Donald Lopez’s The Scientific Buddha is, perhaps, the best example – but often such accounts focus more on pointing out what’s wrong in widespread beliefs about meditation than on what’s (probably, more or less) right.
I have another reason to write about this topic, however, and that is the role of mental imagery in meditation. In most (but not all) kinds of Buddhist meditation,1 mental imagery – roughly, seeing something in “the mind’s eye” – plays a key role. However, there are people – estimates are typically in the 3%~5% range – who do not, and cannot experience mental imagery. This is nowadays called “aphantasia”. The term was coined by Adam Zeman and colleagues in 2015,2 and since then much research has been done about the phenomenon. What’s important to note is that although visual mental imagery is the paradigmatic kind of imagery, there also is auditory imagery (imagining sound), motor imagery (imagining moving a body part), and so forth, and that not all aphantasics lack the exact same kinds of imagery.3
Two years ago, I published a paper about aphantasia, myself.4 What I didn’t mention in that paper – and what I actually haven’t told anyone, except my wife and one or two other people – is that I am aphantasic myself. One reason is that until fairly recently the typical response by philosophers to admitting that one doesn’t experience imagery was one of hostility. Bill Faw, an aphantasic himself, observed that “much of the current imaging literature either denies the existence of wakeful non-mental imagers, [or] views non-imagers motivationally as ‘repressors’ or ‘neurotic’”, for example.5 That I mention my aphantasia here is related to something else I have never publicly spoken or written about before: my experiences with meditation.
That I have tried meditation might not be a surprise to anyone who is familiar with my writings – Buddhist philosophy is one of the recurring themes therein. I have been interested in Buddhism – Buddhist philosophy, especially – since I was a teenager, so it seemed only natural (to me at least) to explore Buddhist meditation a bit as well. That I have never talked or written about this is partially because I consider it somewhat distasteful to do so, but also because I saw little value in doing so. Traditionally, it is more or less taboo to speak about one’s own meditation experience. Whatever the traditional reasons are for that taboo doesn’t need to concern us here, but it must be acknowledged that from an objective point of view, there may not be much value in disclosing what one experienced – or what one believes to have experienced – in meditation. An obvious reason for this is that there is no way to check whether the meditator is giving a truthful account of their experiences, but much more important than that is that it is highly doubtful that the meditator even really knows and understands those experiences themselves. Contrary to what is commonly assumed, we are very bad at observing and understanding what goes on in our own minds,6 and we fill in the gaps in our understanding by means of the explanatory and interpretative tools available to us – that is, by means of relevant ideas in our worldviews and whatever else we believe about meditation and (our own) minds. This makes personal, introspective accounts of meditation almost worthless, even if the meditator does their best to give a truthful account. “Almost”, because even if such an account is colored and distorted, it is still based on something the meditator really experienced.
Despite my hesitations, I will disclose aspects of my own experience with meditation in this article. My reason for that is that I have recently become interested in the role of imagery in meditation and how aphantasic meditators deal with this, which is a topic that, as far as I know, no one else has written about yet.7 I’m not aware of any other detailed description of meditative experiences by an aphantasic (accessible to me), and consequently, my own experiences are all I can go on.8 I am not an experienced meditator, however, so there’s not much I might learn about the effects of aphantasia on/in meditation from just reflecting on my own limited (and probably rather idiosyncratic) experiences. For that reason, I’d be interested in learning about the experiences of other (especially more experienced) aphantasic meditators.
As mentioned in the first paragraph, discussing how aphantasia affects meditation is not the only objective of this article – I also aim to give a very general and sketchy overview of Buddhist meditation and (thereby) remove some common misunderstandings. If you’re only interested in that topic, you can just skip the more personal sections. (I’m by no means an expert on the topic, however, so if you’re seriously interested in Buddhist meditation, I recommend you to read other sources about the topic as well.)
Comment (Feb. 28, 2022)
This may very well be the worst article on this blog (thus far), and partially for that reason I’m considering to delete it.
It is never a good idea to mix purposes and this article illustrates why that is the case. As explained above, I had two purposes for writing and publishing it: (1) to give a brief and general introduction into the topic of meditation in Buddhism, and (2) to contribute to our understanding of the “problem” (if it is one) of aphantasia in meditation. There are better introductions available, however, and moreover, the second purpose gets in the way. The sections in this article that focus exclusively on my own experience with meditation are useless distractions (at best) for anyone who is interested in the general topic, and because of that, the article utterly fails to accomplish its first purpose. In an attempt to improve that, I have put the “personal reflections” in spoilers today, so anyone who is just interested in the general topic can easily skip those. But still, as mentioned, there are better general introductions available. The Youtube channel Religion for Breakfast posted a good, short video on the topic recently, for example.
What’s worse is that the article doesn’t accomplish its second purpose either for a number of reasons. The second personal reflection (on saṃvega-related meditation) has nothing to do with aphantasia and, thus, is out of place here. And the first personal reflection (on dhyāna meditation) provides just a single point of flawed data that can be interpreted in many different ways, and that is, for that reason, unlikely to provide any useful insights. As mentioned above, there is good reason to doubt the reliability of introspection. People tend to believe that they have a pretty good understanding of themselves and what goes on in their minds, but research in psychology and related fields has shown that this is an illusion. Furthermore, we also know now that memory is very unreliable. Consequently, I cannot take my own experiences (and especially memories thereof) for granted and should treat them as critically as I would treat reports of the experiences of others (or probably more critically even). If I do that, I have to conclude that whatever I thought to have experienced in meditation could just as well be some kind of illusion – I just don’t have sufficient reason or “evidence” to discard that skeptical hypothesis. And if that is the case, neither I, nor anyone else can learn anything from my personal reflections.
As mentioned above, I feel deeply comfortable about publicly discussing my personal experiences with meditation, and considerations like those in the previous paragraph only make me feel more uncomfortable. Actually, several months passed between I started writing this article and when I finally “published” it in October 2021, which is entirely due to that discomfort. The more I reflect on it, the more that discomfort grows. Since it doesn’t accomplish either of its two purposes, the article effectively has no purpose, moreover. So it is both useless and unpleasant. It is for that reason that I’m considering deleting it.
Update (March 2, 2022)
After several readers of this blog urged me not to delete this post, I have decided to leave it up. (At least, for now.)
Some brief historical remarks
We do not really know what the Buddha taught. We have the Pāli canon as a source, of course, but the Pāli canon was formed and redacted in a sectarian struggle that lasted centuries and wasn’t completely finalized until king Parakkamabāhu I of Sri Lanka intervened in favor of one sect and one version of the canon in the 12th century.9 Nevertheless, the content of the Pāli canon was pretty much finalized by the 5th century, and we do have some fragments of the lost canons of other sects and other earlier text fragments. These are by no means sufficient to sketch a reliable, detailed picture of the Buddha’s life and teachings, but they give us some clues.
First of all, there are good reasons to believe that the Buddha’s initial teaching was some kind of meditation,10 possibly something like the śamatha or tranquility meditation discussed below. However, there are also good reasons to believe that the idea of a liberating insight or liberating knowledge is of later date and was an invention of early Buddhists in response to a similar idea in then culturally dominant Brahmanism.11 For this reason, it is doubtful that certain kinds of meditation aimed at such liberating insight or knowledge were advocated by the historical Buddha. This doesn’t make them any less “Buddhist”, however, because it is a mistake to think that “Buddhism” is just the teachings of the historical Buddha. The Buddha founded the historical tradition, but the tradition is much more than the founder. Western science and philosophy isn’t identical with (or reducible to) the ideas of its founder, Thales, either.
Although it is almost certain (that is, as certain as we can be) that meditation played a central role in the (initial) teaching of the (historical) Buddha, it is not the case that meditation continued to play such a central role. Given historical evidence, it seems likely that this centrality was lost very early in favor of ritual (such as circumambulating stupas) and study,12 and around 35BCE a debate between those prioritizing meditation and those preferring doctrine was formally decided in favor of the latter.13 Since then, meditation never played a major role in Buddhism (as a whole, even though there were occasional flare-ups of meditative activity within particular sects),14until the early 20th century.
In the late 19th century Buddhist reform movements sprang up in Ceylon/Sri Lanka and Japan, aiming to modernize Buddhism in response to colonialism and Western influence. One aspect of this “Buddhist modernism” is a secularization of Buddhism, that is, an attempt to present Buddhism as a modern, secular “philosophy”, rather than as an outdated, irrational “religion”. This modernization took different forms, but the one that eventually won the struggle between “modernized” Buddhisms was one that heavily emphasized meditative experience.15 It was this kind of Buddhism that became most influential in the West, and consequently, the Western image of Buddhism is heavily distorted by Western/modernized Zen Buddhism, the (originally Burmese) Vipassanā movement, and other modernist currents. The view on meditation of these movements are relatively new and don’t represent Buddhism as a whole (or even a majority view), however.
What is important to realize is that throughout history (with a possible exception for the lifetime of the historical Buddha) only a small minority of Buddhist monks meditated, and that laymen almost never meditated. Even right now, meditation is only important in some Buddhist sects. Most monks spend their time doing rituals and studying doctrine, and Buddhist laymen pray and make offerings. Consequenty, one doesn’t have to meditate to be a Buddhist.16 (But one probably has to meditate to be a Buddhist modernist.)
The main kinds of Buddhist meditation
There is no such thing as Buddhist meditation. Rather, there is a confusing jumble of rituals, techniques, and practices that fall under the various categories that have been translated as “meditation”. Donald Lopez writes in his aforementioned “primer” that:
Over the course of centuries, thousands of meditative practices have developed across the Buddhist cultures of Asia, and a great many of these deviate widely from the notion of a monk seated cross-legged absorbed in a state of silent insight into the ultimate truth.17
Indeed, one doesn’t even have to sit to meditate, as there are also forms of “walking meditation”, and advanced practitioners can supposedly enter into various meditative states regardless of their body posture.
Within this jumble of thousands of practices, traditionally two main categories are discerned, although the boundary between them is vague and there are kinds of meditation that seem to fall outside this classification. The two categories are śamatha (Sanskrit; Pāli: samatha; Chinese: 止) and vipaśyanā (vipassanā; 觀), respectively. The former is usually translated as “tranquility” or “serenity” meditation; the latter as “insight” meditation. The title of Zhiyi’s 智顗 influential 摩訶止觀 is often translated as “The Great Calming and Contemplation” and indeed, “calming” and “contemplation” also occur as descriptive translations of śamatha and vipaśyanā, respectively. I will use the terms “tranquility meditation” and “insight meditation” in the following, but occasionally refer to these other translations. A few other kinds of meditation will be introduced after discussion of these two “main” categories.
Tranquility meditation is not a single technique, but a collection of meditation practices aimed at achieving states of “concentration” called dhyānas (jhānas). These techniques differ by what they take to be the object of meditation (i.e. that what they meditate on) and by which stage or dhyāna in the series of eight (or nine) one aims to reach. I will give a more detailed explanation of these stages/dhyānas below. The goal of tranquility meditation is to develop a kind of “calming” of the mind. (Hence the Chinese term 止 shi, which is often translated as “calming”, but more literally means “stopping”.) This calming and its associated ability to concentrate is supposedly necessary to acquire insight in insight meditation, but not all sources agree about this. While tranquility or a certain stage/dhyāna thereof most often seems to be a prerequisite for gaining a certain kind of insight (in another practice), sometimes it is suggested that tranquility meditation can generate that insight itself. Additionally, “calming” is also supposed to destroy the kleśas, mental afflictions or negative emotions such as ignorance, attachment (or craving, desire, etc.), and aversion (or hatred).
Like tranquility meditation, insight meditation is not a single technique either – if it is a technique at all.18 The main aim of insight meditation is to gain insight into three Buddhist doctrines about the nature of reality: impermanence, no-self (or non-self), and (the nature and causes of) suffering. Supposedly, this insight (ultimately!) leads to liberation from suffering (i.e. nirvāṇa/nibbāna), but also to destruction of the kleśas. It is worth noticing that the Chinese character for insight meditation, 觀 guan, is often translated as “contemplation” because what we commonly understand as contemplation – that is, thinking deeply about something – is indeed included in the category of insight meditation. However, in addition to such discursive/conceptual contemplation, the category also includes techniques that are aimed at seeing (another possible translation of 觀) ultimate reality or some aspect thereof directly. There are, however, considerable differences between schools and sects of Buddhism about what this means, about what would be seen, experienced, or realized in that state, and about how to do this.
Much, but not all, of this difference has to do with different metaphysical views. If insight meditation is also supposed to result in insight in the nature of ultimate reality, then different views about ultimate reality have different implications for the methods, results, and limits of insight meditation. Buddhist views on ultimate reality range from the radically apophatic as in Mādhyamaka to the moderately kataphatic as in, for example, Tiantai 天台 and some of its offshoots. If ultimate reality is completely beyond conceptual description (as in the apophatic view), then this has very different implications than if language can at least be partially right or point us in the right direction (as in a moderately kataphatic view). In the first case, contemplation in the common sense of that term can be of little use, but in the second case, it may be a very useful form of insight meditation. Even then, mere contemplation can never be sufficient because no amount of rational contemplation can produce real insight in no-self (if that is understood as a denial of personal essences, which is not the only possible interpretation). This is because in all of our experience, our apparent/phenomenal self is undeniably there at the center of our experience and it is thus very counter-intuitive to deny that there really is a self. Breaking through this intuitive and phenomenological barrier is not likely to be possible by rational contemplation alone.
An observant reader has probably noted that the standard classification is a bit of a mess. While the defining purpose of insight meditation is insight in the nature of reality, in some understandings tranquility meditation produces that same insight, which seems to erase the boundary between the two categories. Furthermore, both kinds of meditation are supposed to destroy the kleśas. Indeed, in the 20th century, this ambiguity only became worse. In modernist/Western Zen – which takes its name from the Chinese translation of dhyāna, 禪 Chan – it is typically assumed that tranquility meditation leads to insight into the nature of reality (and possibly even to something like liberation). And the Vipassanā movement interprets insight meditation as a kind of technique (or series of techniques) that looks more like tranquility meditation than like traditional practices aimed at gaining insight about impermanence, no-self, and suffering. Because of this, the traditional classification has become effectively meaningless in the context of modern(-ist) meditation practices.
Mindfulness (smṛti)
In the modern, Western understanding, Buddhist meditation is most often associated with mindfulness (smṛti/sati). Mindfulness is not itself a type of Buddhist meditation, however. Rather, it is an aspect of meditation, among others. Mindfulness is the ability to focus one’s undivided attention on something (and to preserve that focus for a substantial stretch of time). Furthermore, smṛti also means memory,19 and in the context of meditation, mindfulness is not necessarily (or even not typically) focused on something that is actually there, but instead on something that is merely imagined on the basis of one’s memory. Mindfulness, then, is typically the ability to focus one’s attention on mental imagery (which can be visual, or auditory, or in any other sense modality) and keep that focus. A “total aphantasic”, who doesn’t have mental imagery in any sense modality, would, thus, be incapable of this kind of mindfulness (but would still be able of mindfulness focused on mind-external, physical objects).
The concept of “mindfulness” has drifted away from its Buddhist (or even older) roots, however, and has become something like a goal in itself in certain therapeutic practices (or a means to achieve certain therapeutic ends). (Whether “mindfulness” really means the same thing in that context is debatable, by the way, but I’ll ignore that problem here.) Some people consider such practices a form of Buddhist meditation and see scientific research thereon as a kind of confirmation that Buddhist meditation “works” or is “true”. I’m a bit more skeptical about this for a number of reasons. Firstly, I think that in most research on meditation, the practices studied and their goals have fairly little to do with Buddhist meditation practices, even taking the immense variety thereof into account. There may be superficial similarities and genetic relations, but they are just not sufficient to translate scientific findings about the beneficial effects of modern “mindfulness” meditation to Buddhist tranquility meditation, for example. (Even if mindfulness in its original sense is an essential part of tranquility meditation.) To me, assuming an essential similarity between modern “mindfulness” meditation and Buddhist meditation practices is missing the point (or the many points) of Buddhist meditation.
Secondly, there are major methodological problems and hurdles in research on the supposed beneficial effects of meditation. One problem is that in most (perhaps, even all) studies that I have seen at least some of the researchers involved already believed in the beneficence of meditation prior to the research, resulting in a serious risk of confirmation bias. A meta-analysis is needed to test whether prior beliefs of the researchers significantly influence research results, but as far as I know, this has not been done yet.20 More important than this even, is that it is extremely difficult to distinguish real effects of meditation – if there are such effects – from placebo effects. It seems that the only way to do this would be to divide a group of research subjects in two; teach one group a certain meditation technique and the other some “fake” technique; then mix them back together and let researchers who don’t know whether what the subjects are doing is “real” or “fake” do the testing; and finally let other researchers who do know who did what do the final analysis. (This would be the most obvious way of doing the kind of double-blind research necessary to exclude placebo effects, but perhaps there are other options.) Doing something like this would be very difficult, and there might even be ethical objections. But as long as something like this isn’t done, we have no way of knowing whether supposed beneficial effects of meditation are real or mere placebo effects.
Thirdly, I don’t see much value in certain kinds of physiological/neurological research on “real” Buddhist meditation (rather than modern mindfulness and other offshoots). An fMRI scan may show that certain brain areas are more or less active in a meditator’s brain, which might be interesting from a cognitive science point of view and might tell us something about how the brain works. But I don’t see what we can learn about meditation itself from that. It’s the phenomenological qualities of the meditative experience that matter, and those you cannot see on an fMRI scan. Those can only be experienced in meditation itself.21
Tranquility meditation and the dhyānas
Let’s now turn our attention to tranquility meditation. As explained above, the goal of this kind of meditation is to progress through a number of stages called dhyānas (jhānas in Pāli). There is some variety in the interpretation of these stages (and there are variants of, and levels within the stages as well), but I will follow Roderick Bucknell’s account here, as that is based on both a thorough textual analysis of relevant sources and actual meditative experience by practitioners (and as it makes most sense to me, partially because it roughly matches some of my own experiences – see next section).22 (If you’re seriously interested in the topic, there are plenty of sources that give much more detailed accounts of these stages and the most common experiences associated with them, but note that some of those descriptions lean heavily on mental imagery.)
supplemental note (Oct. 26)
It was brought to my attention after publishing this that in some interpretations the meditation object can also be a visualization – that is a mental image. It’s not clear to me, however, why the first four dhyānas are considered “material” if this is right.
There are eight dhyānas in total (or four plus four “formless attainments” – there is some disagreement about whether the latter should be considered dhyānas or something else, but I’ll include them in the list of dhyānas here.23) The first four are usually called “material dhyānas” and the last four “immaterial dhyānas” (or “formless dhyānas”), because the first four involve a material or physical meditation object (while the latter four don’t). Traditionally that object was a clay disc, nowadays it is usually one’s breath, but it really can be anything. Thus far, I have suggested that the dhyānas are like stages, but they are probably better understood as the goals of subsequent stages of tranquility meditation, and in the following I will separate these stages and their goals.
In the first stage, the meditator focuses their attention on the meditation object, but fails to (completely) stop normal thought (vitarka-vicāra). Initially, the meditator gets distracted by their though processes soon and forcibly returns their attention to the meditation object every time, but with continuing practice, they can keep their attention on the object longer and longer. Typically, this practice results in a kind of pleasant sensation called prīti (pīti in Pāli). The first dhyāna is characterized by the presence of the “mental factors” mentioned, vitarka-vicāra and prīti, as well as a third, sukha, which refers (here) to a different kind of pleasure or happiness that is less sensual, less exciting, and less fleeting than prīti. The second to fourth dhyānas are defined by the cessation of these mental factors. Vitarka-vicāra ceases in the second; prīti in the third; and sukha in the fourth.
In the second stage, as already implied by the definition of the second dhyāna given above, conscious thought stops, and the meditator is completely focused on the meditation object, resulting in a pleasant kind of calmness. Many meditators experience bodily reactions, such as trembling or goose bumps that may distract them and break their concentration. With continuing practice, these side-effects disappear.
The main (i.e. most important) characteristic of the third stage is a further increase in the ability to focus one’s attention, but with increasingly less effort. Like the second stage, the third has various side-effects, such as a sensation of warmth or lightness, or even the illusion that one is floating. (These sensations are sometimes referred to as sukha felt by the body.)
In the fourth stage, these side-effects disappear, leaving nothing in the meditator’s consciousness except the meditation object. At this stage, it seems as if that meditation object is the only thing that exists.
After this, the material/physical meditation object as the focus of attention drops out of the picture. In the fifth stage, it is replaced by a mental image thereof, which can take varying forms. Typically, the mental image is not a mere mental representation of the original material/physical meditation object. Instead of “seeing” the original object in the mind’s eye, the meditator might, for example, see a patch of light with the same shape. In the sixth stage, this mental image becomes more abstract, but also more stable and vivid. Both the fifth and sixth stage also come with other sensations. In case of the fifth, that is a sensation of infinite space; in case of the sixth, it is a sensation of infinite consciousness. (But I must admit, that I don’t really understand what “infinite consciousness” means.) It is these sensations that define the fifth and sixth dhyānas, respectively
In the seventh stage, the abstract mental image disappears and there is nothing left in consciousness. This is sometimes described as a kind of black nothingness. It is important to realize that what the meditator experiences in this stage is nothingness (ākiṃcanyā/ ākiñcañña) and not emptiness (śūnyatā/suññatā), which is something else entirely. Nothingness is just the complete emptying of the conscious mind. Emptiness is the nature of ultimate reality. What exactly that means differs between sects and schools, but most often it refers to something like the idea that ultimate reality doesn’t (exactly) correspond to how we normally experience the world around us. Our conscious experience of reality is cut up into things and kinds based on our concepts and beliefs, for example, but ultimate reality isn’t like that. Again, there is significant variation with regards to such metaphysical ideas, and metaphysics isn’t the topic here, but it is important to realize that experiencing nothingness is not the same (or even related to) experiencing emptiness. These are – again – two very different things.
(I wonder, by the way, whether the idea that tranquility meditation can also produce insight into the nature of reality is based on a mistaken identification of nothingness with emptiness, but I haven’t really looked into this. Another possible mistake that could have lead to this idea is that because at this stage nothing exists in consciousness, nothing really exist in ultimate reality either. This conclusion, obviously, doesn’t follow. Neither does the non-experience of a self in this state teach anything about no-self/non-self. It is, perhaps, worth noting that in the Dhātuvibhanga Sutta (MN14), the Buddha says that the nothingness etcetera experienced in tranquility meditation is “conditioned”, meaning that it is not ultimately real and implying – in the context of the passage – that it is created by the mind. From this it also follows that tranquility meditation cannot give insight into (the nature of) reality.24)
Finally, in the eighth stage, consciousness disappears. This is obviously a state that the meditator cannot experience (because they are unconscious) and that can only be inferred afterwards due to some indication of the passage of time. I haven’t mentioned the seventh and eighth dhyānas yet, but those are defined by exactly these effects: the experience of nothingness in case of the seventh and unconsciousness in case of the eighth.
Supplement (Feb. 28, 2022)
Doug Smith has recently posted a series of videos on his Youtube channel Doug’s Dharma about various aspects of dhyāna meditation. There is a series of videos on the first, second, third, and fourth, respectively; as well as videos on the formless attainments (i.e. dhyānas 5 to 8); on what may be the ninth; and on the question how manydhyānas there were in “early Buddhism”.
Some personal reflections
In the introduction of this article, I mentioned some of my reasons to be very hesitant about disclosing my own experiences with meditation. One reason that I didn’t mention there is that some aspects of my experience are nearly impossible to express in words, which has a lot to do with my aphantasia. I’m near-total aphantasic – that is, I don’t experience mental imagery in any sense modality, except one, but that one is very hard to explain.
There isn’t really such a thing as visual mental imagery, but rather, there are two kinds of visual imagery: object imagery and spatial imagery. Object imagery is the ability to see objects in your “mind’s eye” – I cannot do that. Some people who lack object imagery compare it to seeing a black void, but that doesn’t seem entirely accurate to me, because if you’re seeing black in your imagination, you’re still seeing something. There is no blackness in my imagination either – just nothing. However, this nothingness is not the same as the nothingness of the seventh dhyāna, although I have no clue how to explain the difference.
While I don’t have object imagery, I do have some kind of spatial imagery, but what that is like is nearly impossible to explain to anyone who isn’t even aware that these are two different kinds of things. It’s a bit like the ability to imagine locations in space, without there being anything at those locations, or – in a sense – without there being space, even. These locations aren’t really points either, because that still connotes a kind of “things”. So, while I cannot imagine a triangle, I can (in some sense) imagine its three vertices. I don’t “see” them, however. They are bare locations, and moreover, they are relative locations, relative to each other and to (almost!) nothing else. What’s really weird – even to me – is that this spatial imagery doesn’t map to the real world, but feels to be located outside me anyway. When I imagine the three vertices of a triangle, it feels as if they are about 30 to 40 cm in front of me, and about 10 to 20 cm above eye level. I don’t “see” them there – because I don’t see anything in imagination – but it kind of feels as if that’s where the three vertices are. There are just vertices, by the way; no lines. My spatial imagery is limited to imagining very small numbers of relative locations (and I can only focus on one of those locations at a time), but that’s all. And again, there is nothing at those locations.
Let’s turn to some of my experiences with meditation now. Where the above plays a role will become evident soon enough. Before proceeding, I must emphasize that expressing some of these experiences in words is equally difficult, and that my description might not be completely reliable for a number of reasons (some of which I already mentioned above). Firstly, I can only describe these experiences on the basis of what I remember and some notes in old notebooks (which I fortunately kept, but which are a disorganized mess, so I surely missed some relevant notes), and my autobiographical memory is terrible. This is not uncommon for aphantasics and has been given the official name “Severely Deficient Autobiographical Memory” (SDAM).25 My memories that I have encoded conceptually in sufficient detail and that I have retrieved often enough to prevent memory loss tend to be fairly reliable, but this requires conscious processes and I have doubtlessly lost far more memories than I have retained. Secondly, my interpretation of these experiences is colored by relevant beliefs and expectations, both at the time that I first contemplated and conceptualized them and stored them in my memory, and in later retrievals of those memories, including now.
As mentioned, I first became interested in Buddhism in the middle of the 1980s when I was a teenager, but I didn’t much pursue that interest at the time, and only started to seriously engage with Buddhism until after moving to Japan in early 2008. That’s not where my experience with meditation starts, however, although I didn’t think of my earlier experiences as “meditation” at the time. Some time in the second half of the 1990s, when I was in my late 20s, I developed a kind of relaxation or calming method by myself. I would lay down on my back on a bed or on the floor and focus on my heartbeat. I think that what first gave me this idea is the sensation of a fast-beating hard when one is agitated or otherwise not calm. At first, it was pretty difficult to even focus on my heartbeat, but as I did this quite often for some reason, I got better at it soon. After a while, I could focus on my heartbeat without any thoughts intervening for up to about 10 minutes. I even developed the ability to slightly speed up or down my heartbeat – or so I thought, at least, because retrospectively I suspect that this was mere illusion. As mentioned, I didn’t think about this as “meditation” at the time; just as a pleasant technique to calm myself down and increase my ability to concentrate or to sleep better. But thinking about it later, it looks very much like tranquility meditation. Both the posture and meditation object may be unusual, but aside from that I see little difference. In terms of the descriptions given in the previous section, my experiences correspond to those of the first and second dhyānas, including occasional goose bumps (but I don’t remember any other bodily responses).
Around 2005 or so, I started doing this “calming exercise” less and less, and my next experiences with something like tranquility meditation date to 2008 or 2009 (I’m not sure about the exact date). By this time I had rekindled my interest in Buddhism and was studying Buddhist philosophy (among many other things). Based on what I read in books and on the internet, I decided at some point to try meditation. So I sat down in the required posture and tried to focus on my breath and utterly failed. But that’s supposed to happen, of course. At first, it is very hard to keep one’s attention focused, but with continued practice this ability should get stronger and stronger. I didn’t make much progress, however, which is probably largely due to the fact that I didn’t try often enough and didn’t make enough effort. At some point I remembered my earlier “calming exercise”, however, and started wondering why I used to be able to focus my attention on my heartbeat (even though I hadn’t been doing that for a while at the time), but not on my breath. I never really considered making my heartbeat a meditation object, however, and perhaps that would not have been a good idea indeed, but I did copy another aspect of that old “calming exercise”: laying on my back instead of sitting. I don’t know how much that mattered, but what did seem to make a difference was the realization that focusing on my breath flowing in and out of my nose wasn’t actually all that different from focusing on my heartbeat. They are both subtle, bodily rhythms, and in both cases “getting into the groove” was the first step.26
With practice, I slowly got somewhat better at focusing my attention on the air stream in my nose, and I recognize some of the side-effects mentioned by Bucknell: goose bumps (like before), a weird, warm tingling sensation starting in my fingers and toes, and a floating feeling. At a number of occasions, it felt as if I was floating above my own body. (Remember that I was laying on my back, while doing this. Lacking any kind of mystical or spiritual instincts, I considered such floating sensations irrelevant illusions, by the way, although I remember them as feeling quite pleasant.) With further practice all such sensations disappeared and whatever I had been focusing my attention on was the only thing left. That wasn’t always my breath (or more specifically, the air flowing out and into my nose), as I also experimented with other meditation objects.
As explained above, I have spatial imagery, meaning that I can (sort of) imagine small sets of relative locations (without seeing them – this really feels like something completely different from all other sense modalities). However, because it feels as if these locations are imagined in a virtual space in front of me, they are to some extent not just spatially related to each other, but also to me. The spatial relations between the imagined locations are different from their spatial relation to me, but I cannot explain this difference. These locations relative to me are important, however, because it is this aspect of my spatial imagery that allows me to focus my attention on the imaginary space in which I place these locations itself, and collapse that imaginary space into a single point. That point “feels” as if it is roughly in the location in front of me that I mentioned before, but I don’t see, sense, or imagine anything to be there. It is just an entirely abstract location or point in space, and moreover, it is not really a location in space outside me – it is not really there in front of me – there merely is some kind of strange association with the property of being in front of me without it actually having that property. So it is there and it isn’t there and there is nothing there except the thereness itself, which isn’t really there either. I said that this stuff is hard to explain, didn’t I?…
Anyway, I used this point in space or non-space or whatever as a meditation object as well, and that actually worked much better than my breath or other things I tried. But when I think back about this now, and try to make sense of my experiences, I’m not sure how to do so. When I map my experiences with meditation focused on my breath to the stages and dhyānas explained in the previous section, it seems to me that I reached the fourth stage and associated dhyāna in which nothing but the meditation object is left. I wasn’t very good at this, as I rarely achieved that state for more than a few minutes and often failed altogether, but there have been a number occasions that retrospectively seem to have been examples of the fourth dhyāna. Moving forward from there would have been impossible, however, as the fifth and sixth stages (and associated dhyānas) require mental imagery.27 Thus far, things seem relatively straightforward, although I emphasize once more that I do not completely trust my own memories.
The problem is interpreting the meditation experiences that took the aforementioned imaginary “point” as their focus, and those gradually became more frequent than meditations on my breath, as I found it much easier to focus my attention on that “point”. I don’t remember having anything like goose bumps in those meditations, but the out-of-body floating sensation was more often a side-effect of this meditation than of breath-focused meditation. Hence, it might seem that in case of such a floating experience, I would have been at the third stage, but I’m not sure whether that is right. The problem is that the first four stages and dhyānas assume a material/physical meditation object (and only move on to an immaterial, imagined object in the fifth and sixth), but in these meditations, something immaterial and imagined was the focus of meditation from the start, and that meditation object wasn’t even an “object” except, perhaps, in a purely grammatical sense of the term.
supplemental note (Oct. 26)
As mentioned in a supplemental note above, in some interpretations, the meditation object can also be something like a mental image, which might solve this interpretative problem. (Provided that that interpretation is right, of course.)
What, then, was I doing? Aside from the different meditation object, it didn’t feel or seem any different, and probably for that reason, I didn’t even think about the difference at that time. In case of meditation on my breath, I reached a stage in which only the meditation object was there, and in case of the “point”-based meditation I reached (what appeared to be) the exact same stage. (In case of the “point”-based meditation it was also easier – or at least, it became easier with practice – to skip some of the lower-numbered dhyānas and jump straight to what appeared to be the third or fourth.) So does that mean that I reached the fourth dhyāna in these meditations as well? I don’t know. Again, typical descriptions of this dhyāna assume a physical meditation object, which becomes the only thing in one’s consciousness, but there was nothing physical in my consciousness.
It gets even more complicated beyond that. Because of my aphantasia, proceeding to the fifth stage when meditating on a traditional mediation object is impossible, because it is supposed to be replaced by a mental image. However, if my experiences with meditation focused on an imagined location in imaginary space are interpreted in terms of the traditional dhyānas and meditation stages, then I did move on beyond the fourth stage on several occasions. That is, the point disappeared, leaving nothing in its place. This nothingness might seem characteristic of the seventh dhyāna, but that interpretation is surely wrong. As mentioned before, in case of the fifth stage/dhyāna when the meditation object is replaced with an image, this is accompanied by a sense of infinite space, and I did experience that a number of times. At those occasions, that sense of infinite space was the only thing in my conscious mind, as the meditation object had disappeared. So, the experience of infinite space is characteristic of the fifth dhyāna, but the absence of anything else is not.
And then, once,28 that sense of infinite space subsided as well and there was nothing left at all. No “point”, no space, no time, no me, nothing. Just absolute nothingness. This lasted for about 15 minutes (until my wife disturbed me).29 So what does this mean? Did I skip the sixth dhyāna and go straight to the seventh? Is that supposed to be possible? Again, I don’t know. Based on what I remember about this state and what I read about meditation, it appears very much like the seventh dhyāna, but if skipping a step is supposed to be impossible, and if the lower stages must involve a physical mediation object then it can’t be. This experience raises so many questions… I have even wondered whether – if this was the seventh dhyāna – might I have even made it to the eighth? That question is impossible to answer, however, as the eighth stage is an unconscious stage, which can only be inferred from the passage of time. When I was in this state, I didn’t experience the passage of time, however, so I wouldn’t know if there had been a gap or jump explained by unconsciousness. It is for this reason that I am highly suspicious about the eighth stage/dhyāna. Supposedly, it can be achieved, but the meditator can never know whether they achieved it.
Retrospectively, what puzzles me most about this experience is not how to make sense of it within the standard dhyāna framework, but what came after it: utter indifference. I wasn’t even disinterested in tranquility meditation after this meditative experience of nothingness, because that still assumes a kind of awareness and decision to not pursue something. Rather, it seemed like the whole idea of meditation, at least as something I might do, was wiped from my mind. After this experience, it just didn’t occur to me that I might meditate. To this day, I don’t really understand this. Before this experience, I frequently tried to meditate and had a strong (albeit mostly academic) interest in practicing meditation. But it was as if experiencing nothingness had wiped that away completely, so from one day to the next, I completely stopped tranquility meditation. What is strangest about this is that it took years before I even realized how weird this is. Something that somehow mattered to me, suddenly and inexplicably stopped mattering to me, and I even seemed to have forgotten (from one day to the next) that this “thing” used to matter to me. But because I was suddenly indifferent, none of this even occurred to me at the time – as I wrote, it was as if every thought about tranquility meditation was suddenly wiped from my mind.30
Since that day, I never tried tranquility meditation again. It just never occurred to me as something I could do, and I’m not really sure about why I should either. As mentioned before, my main aim (at the time) was to get a better understanding of what tranquility meditation is about. Thus far I’m satisfied with what I learned and see no reason to pursue the subject any further. I have no desire to reach any of these various states again either. I understand why some people are attracted to this kind of meditation, but I feel no desire for any of its effects.31
Well, actually, that is not completely true, because I sometimes do something that could technically be called “tranquility meditation”, but that I don’t really perceive as such. In the Visuddhimagga, Buddhaghosa refuses to define meditation because it comes in many different kinds and because it is unhelpful to try to reign in that diversity by means of a strict definition (III.2). I have always interpreted this as implying that tranquility meditation is a functional category, which means that some practice is a form of tranquility meditation because of its goal or function and not because of aspects of the practice itself. So, when I use meditation-like techniques to better deal with the effects of an oncoming migraine,32 then my goal has nothing to do with the dhyānas, and therefore, even if what I’m doing is exactly like stages one and two of tranquility meditation, it is not actually meditation – to me, at least – but something else entirely.
Comment (Feb. 28, 2022)
As mentioned in my other comment with the same date at the end of the introduction to this article, I have become increasingly skeptical about my own meditative experiences. Although what I can reconstruct on the basis of my notes and memories certainly does seem to correspond with descriptions of various dhyānas that I have found (most of which I wasn’t aware of at the time I meditated, by the way), there are other explanations. What makes me especially suspicious is that it seems that few if any meditators reach stages beyond the second dhyāna on their own. (And I haven’t found a single account of another aphantasic “progressing” beyond the second.) It is, of course, possible (in theory, at least) that I managed to reach the seventh dhyāna without a meditation teacher or other kind of guide (and I discuss one possible explanation in the second addendum below), but it seems quite unlikely, and the more likely explanation is that it was all an illusion. This doesn’t mean that I have concluded that my meditative experiences were indeed illusory – rather, it means that I don’t know. I might have reached the seventh dhyāna on one occasion and the fifth on several, but then again, I might not. I don’t know, and there is no compelling evidence to decide either way. (As mentioned, introspection and memory are not reliable guides.) It is largely for that reason, that these personal reflections are useless. Nothing can be learned from them.
Additional Comment
(Added a few hours after all of the other comments dated February 28.) — I do not doubt that I experienced infinite space and nothingness in meditation. My memory may be unreliable (and so is everyone else’s memory), but I have very clear memories of those experiences (as well as a few notes mentioning/describing them). So what to make of this?
Could there be multiple senses of “infinite space” and “nothingness”, or multiple kinds of experiences of “infinite space” and “nothingness”? I don’t think that suggestion makes sense. Nothing is just nothing. And an empty infinite space is just that. If it’s empty, there is nothing to identify it or to distinguish it.
Could these experiences of infinite space and nothingness be illusions, then? Well, yes. But in the Dhātuvibhanga Sutta (MN14), the Buddha basically says that they are illusions. (He says that such experiences are “conditioned”, which in the given context essentially means that they are produced by the mind and are thus illusory.) So if they are illusions anyway, then that doesn’t disqualify my experiences thereof.
Then, could my memories of those experiences be illusions instead? That’s possible, of course, but as mentioned, these are among the memories I’m most confident about. I’m quite sure that I indeed experienced infinite space and nothingness and that my memories thereof are not illusions.
So what does that mean? If the fifth and seventh dhyānas are defined by these experiences, then this would seem to imply that I “reached” those stages in meditation. However, I remain somewhat skeptical for reasons mentioned above. Or actually, I keep swinging back and forth, unable to make up my mind, unable to decide how to interpret all of this. I’m fine with that uncertainty, except when I would have to address the issue publicly. There are no short and simple answers to questions about my experiences with meditation, and for that reason, to avoid being forced to give simple answers that feel wrong, I try to avoid the topic altogether.
Other kinds of meditation: death and compassion
Not all kinds of meditation are easily classified in the tranquility/insight framework, and some don’t seem to fit altogether. The most prominent kind of meditation in the Tibetan tradition is a kind of ritualized imagination of Buddhas or Buddha lands, for example. In his Buddhism & Science, Donald Lopez spends ten pages giving a very detailed description of the mental visualizations involved in this kind of meditation.41 Robert Sharf is somewhat skeptical that monks engaged in this kind of ritual actually see all of these details in their minds,42 but we’ll leave that aside. Another good example are the brahmavihārās, a set of four kinds of meditation on lovingkindness, compassion, sympathetic joy, and equanimity. (The term brahmavihārā does not just refer to these meditations, but also – and actually more often – to the attitudes that are the intended result thereof.) Obviously, I cannot possibly discuss all kinds of Buddhist meditation here (as mentioned, there are thousands) so I’ll just focus on a few that I find interesting, and Tibetan visualization rituals are not among those.
Two of the most influential and important Buddhist texts dealing with meditation are Buddhaghosa’s Visuddhimagga (in the Theravāda tradition) and Śāntideva’s Bodhicaryāvatāra (in the Mahāyāna tradition). The latter is also one of my favorite books (even though I don’t agree with certain aspects of Śāntideva’s Mādhyamaka philosophy). In the Visuddhimagga, Buddhaghosa writes that there are two meditation subjects that “are needed generally and desirable owing to their great helpfulness” (III.59),43 and thus, that everyone should pursue. These two essential kinds of meditation are the meditations on death and on lovingkindness. (Confusingly, he discusses these in the part of the book on tranquility meditation, but I don’t think they should be classified as such as their goals have nothing to do with the dhyānas.)
Buddhaghosa’s meditation on death is not the well-known meditation on the grossness of the body and/or its decomposition after death. Rather, it is a meditation intended to bring about a full realization of one’s own inevitable death. It is successful only if it results in a state of shock called saṃvega (VIII.5-6 and III.58). What exactly saṃvega is, is nowhere defined clearly, but because I was interested in the topic (partially for personal reasons – see below), I decided to do some research into the nature and supposed effects of saṃvega resulting in a paper published in the Journal of Buddhist Ethics in 2016.44 In that paper I compared saṃvega to descriptions of similar states of shock related to the realization of one’s own inevitable death in Western philosophical literature. The most compelling description I found was by James Baillie:
I entered into a state of mind unlike anything I had experienced before. I realized that I will die. It may be tomorrow, it may be in fifty years time, but one way or another it is inevitable and utterly non-negotiable. I no longer just knew this theoretically, but knew it in my bones. . . . It was as if the blinders had been removed, and I was the only person to have woken up from a collective dream to grasp the terror of the situation.45
The experience of this state of shock matters, according to Buddhaghosa, because it increases religious and moral motivation by decreasing attachments (III.8), and increasing lovingkindness (XIII.35) and vigor (XIV.137). (Arguably, it also gives insight into one’s own impermanence.) Somewhat similarly, Śāntideva writes that “the virtue of suffering has no rival, since, from the shock [saṃvega] it causes, intoxication falls away and there arises compassion for those in cyclic existence, fear of evil, and a longing for the Conqueror [i.e., the Buddha]”.46 How exactly it could have such effects doesn’t need to concern us here, but for a possible explanation see my paper on the topic.47
The goal of Buddhaghosa’s second essential meditation is a gradual extension of lovingkindness from (at first) those close to the meditator until it eventually covers all sentient beings. “Lovingkindness” is a translation of the Pāli word mettā (Sanskrit: maitrī). In older Vedic texts, the Sanskrit term is associated with love and sympathy, and it is also related to compassion. A more common translation of “compassion” is karuṇā, however, and mettā and karuṇā are indeed not synonymous. Buddhaghosa’s explanation of the difference in IX.108-9 is that karuṇā is concerned with alleviating the suffering of others, while mettā is concerned with their general well-being, which seems to imply that compassion is a special kind or aspect of lovingkindness, because concern for the well-being of those who suffer is concern with the alleviation of that suffering.48 On the same grounds, the positive counterpart of karuṇā, muditā, could also be considered a special kind or aspect of mettā. Muditā, sometimes translated as “sympathetic joy”, is sharing in others’ joy or fortune – it is genuine happiness due to the other’s happiness. Taking all of this into account, it makes most sense to me to think of mettā as a virtue related to compassion, and of karuṇā and muditā as emotional attitudes that motivate and support that virtue. This virtue of mettā (lovingkindness) is the genuine desire for the well-being of all sentient beings, for the alleviation of their suffering, and for the enhancement of their happiness and joy. Hence, mettā is a kind of target-less, impartial, and universal compassion.49 This, then, is what I take to be the aim of this “essential” meditation: to develop a sense of impartial compassion for all sentient beings.
A very similar goal can be found in Śāntideva’s Bodhicaryāvatāra. In the last part of his chapter on the perfection of meditation, he introduces “the supreme mystery: exchange of self and other” (8:120):
If one does not let go of self one cannot let go of suffering, as one who does not let go of fire cannot let go of burning. | Therefore, in order to allay my own suffering and to allay the suffering of others, I devote myself to others and accept them as myself. | Hey Mind, make the resolve, “I am bound to others”! From now on you must have no other concern than the welfare of all beings.50
The placement near the end of the chapter (as well as the argument leading up to it) suggests that this is the aim of the perfection of meditation – it is, thus, the highest form of meditation, and it is aimed at developing the virtuous attitude of a Bodhisattva, who doesn’t hesitate to sacrifice himself to save others.
Those who have developed the continuum of their mind in this way, to whom the suffering of others is as important as the things they themselves hold dear, plunge down in the Avīci hell as geese into a cluster of lotus blossoms.51
Further personal reflections
Saṃvega is typically assumed to be triggered by something like contemplation of your own inevitable death (and Buddhaghosa appears to make that assumption indeed), but my first experience of the state was triggered by a news photograph of carbonized bodies of a family in a burnt-out car in a war zone. I was a teenager at the time, and briefly I was overwhelmed with the fear I imagine those people must have felt in the moments before they were murdered, followed by an intense state of shock, which in every respect matches descriptions of saṃvega-like states like those by James Baillie. I was a teenager at the time, and this experience was somewhat traumatic. It took weeks before the sense of intense discomfort wore off (and months until it disappeared completely). Thankfully, being aphantasic actually made that easier, because not being able to experience imagery also made it impossible to recall the picture itself. (It is also for this reason that aphantasics appear to be less prone to depression and PTSD, by the way.)
The next time I experienced saṃvega was more typical, but I don’t remember when exactly that took place. I’m not sure either whether I first read James Baillie’s paper about the topic, or Buddhaghosa’s recommendation of the meditation on death, but I think it was the second. In any case, when I read the relevant parts of the Visuddhimagga I was immediately reminded of my own saṃvega-like experiences, and I found it very easy to bring about that state again. (I’m pretty sure that this was some time after I had stopped tranquility meditation, but not much later, so in practice it seems that I didn’t really stop meditating – although there may have been a short break – but merely switched from one kind of meditation to another.)
In reading Buddhaghosa, I noticed that the meditation on death is also supposed to lead to lovingkindness, which lead me to wonder whether his two “essential” meditations are necessarily separate. If one practices to achieve saṃvega triggered by the suffering and/or death of others, could one do both kinds of meditation at once? I don’t and didn’t know the answer to that question. And neither do I know how to practice Śāntideva’s “supreme mystery”, but somehow all of these gave rise to a personal and highly idiosyncratic meditation practice that some meditators might not recognize as meditation at all.
The core of that practice – at least in its initial phase – was to “share” in the terror felt by victims of extreme suffering leading to death as reported in the news. Every morning, when reading the news, I would pick one news story – and there would be plenty of horror to choose from every day – and meditated on that. I suppose that some explanation is in order here. Being aphantasic, I cannot use imagery to do something like this. There is obviously a kind of imagination involved, but no imagery of any kind. Neither did or do I place myself in the position of the suffering other and then imagine what I would feel like. Instead, I tried to “feel” the other’s terror and suffering (as if I were that other), to experience the other’s realization that they were going to die, to share in all the pain, fear, and suffering. I tried … until it hit, like a fist in the stomach, and my whole body and mind were incapacitated by unadulterated pain and terror.
So why would anyone do that, you may ask. Why would anyone voluntarily go through something like that every morning? (Except on weekends, because I could only do this when I was alone.) The answer to that question is simple: because I thought and still think that this kind of exercise is valuable. It’s a kind of daily reminder of what really matters. It gives me insight into suffering (or so I like to think, at least), it makes me more compassionate (I hope), and it helps me better understand or realize no-self (as well as some aspects of impermanence). As such, this practice could be understood as a kind of insight meditation as well. Recall that insight meditation is aimed at gaining insight about suffering, no-self, and impermanence, and so is this meditation practice.
My aim of this idiosyncratic practice was to make a saṃvega-like shock in response to (reports of) the suffering and/or death of others more or less automatic, but also somewhat more controllable and less traumatic. It didn’t take too long to succeed in that respect, but it required some changes in the way I consumed the daily news. There was a period, for example, when reading the news would invariably produce states of shock, because that response was indeed getting more or less automatic, but I still had very limited ability to control that shock. Obviously, this kind of shock is very stressful, and on most mornings, after saṃvega had worn off, it would find myself crying. (This is not a crying out of sadness, however, but a more physical response to intense stress.) Even after learning to better prepare myself beforehand, and to better control saṃvega and reign in its effects, this doesn’t always work. I remember that one time, three years ago, I was reading a report by a human rights organization about the war in Rwanda in the 1990s with rather detailed accounts of some of the atrocities. Although I thought that I had “armed” and prepared my mind, I was suddenly hit by saṃvega, and after the initial shock had disappeared, I started crying uncontrollably. In a crowded Japanese commuter train.
Nowadays, I’m rarely hit by the full force of saṃvega. I suppose it really got blunted. And because of that, this practice is starting to lose some of its value. It won’t result in the “exchange of self and other” – that is, in genuinely caring (at least) as much about the suffering and happiness of others as I care about myself. I’m not aiming to become a Bodhisattva (and I know myself well enough to realize that I would never succeed anyway), so that might not seem to matter much, but I want to get at least somewhat closer to that ideal. That desire is itself (continuously) reinforced by this practice, by the way, so I consider that an important positive effect, at least.
So what does my current “meditation” practice consist of? Well, it depends on how you define “meditation”. I don’t do tranquility meditation anymore (unless you count the use of something very much like that to better deal with migraine). I regularly do insight meditation if you count discursive contemplation on relevant metaphysical doctrines, but that is far removed from typical, modern understandings of “meditation” (but not from a more classical understanding as expressed in, for example, Descartes’s “Meditations”). In a sense, I still “meditate” on death and lovingkindness/compassion in my weird idiosyncratic way, but that practice doesn’t look anything like stereotypes of meditation either. Additionally, I sometimes meditate on Thich Nhat Hanh’s famous poem Please Call Me by My True Names,52 but that is really a variant of the same idiosyncratic practice.53 So the answer to the question whether I meditate or not depends very much on one’s definition of “meditation”. I have no definition, so I have no clear answer to that question either.
Concluding remarks
There’s one question that some readers may want to ask that is worth addressing here: Do I consider myself a Buddhist? I have no easy answer to that question, however. I don’t call myself a Buddhist because calling oneself thus creates images and expectations in the minds of others that don’t really correspond to reality. I don’t believe in karma, rebirth, or Pure Lands. I don’t sit cross-legged in tranquility meditation or one of its variants or offshoots. I don’t pray to Buddha statues. And so forth. I’m not even religious.
In Japanese and Chinese the equivalent of the English term “Buddhist” is 佛教徒, which literally translates as “student/disciple of (the) Buddhist teachings”.56 This could be a term I’m more comfortable with: I’m certainly a student of Buddhist teachings (but maybe not a “disciple” – I’m too much of an intellectual anarchist for that). However, these terms have the exact same problematic connotations (i.e. belief in rebirth in a Pure Land, etc.) in practice.
Furthermore, I don’t think it would be appropriate to call myself a “Buddhist” for another reason: even though my philosophical ideas are heavily influenced by Buddhist philosophy, I do not unconditionally accept any doctrine or teaching, but only those that I believe to be sufficiently supported by evidence and/or argument. And even those, I only accept provisionally. I’m something like a Quinean naturalist or pragmatist first, and if I am a Buddhist at all, I’d only be a Buddhist second. (So maybe I’m a “Buddhist naturalist”? But I don’t feel entirely comfortable with that label either.)
Anyway, the question how I identify myself ideologically/philosophically isn’t really that interesting. What I find much more interesting is whether there are other aphantasics who have meditative experiences similar to mine, or very different ones. But also whether there are others (aphantasic or not) who developed their own idiosyncratic meditation practices to gain better insight in no-self and suffering and/or to strengthen lovingkindness and compassion (regardless of whether I actually succeeded in these goals), like I described in the previous section.
In addition to that, another goal I had in writing this article is to explain that Buddhist meditation is much broader and varied than often assumed. I have only scratched the surface here – Buddhaghosa alone describes or mentions tens or even hundreds of meditations (depending on how one counts). And lastly, I want to emphasize once more that one doesn’t even have to meditate to be a Buddhist. There are millions of Buddhists who never meditate, but who instead pray or make offerings or perform rituals or study. All of those people legitimately call themselves “Buddhists”. I’m not going to disagree with that designation – I have a hard enough time figuring out my own.
Addendum (November 2nd)
In the first week after posting this article, several people have responded to it, sharing their own experiences or suggesting potentially relevant research. Many (but not nearly all) of the people who responded mentioned that they are aphantasic themselves, and some described experiences quite similar to mine. I haven’t received a response from an open-minded, experienced meditator yet, but that is surely something I would also be interested in.
Among the personal experiences shared by others one of the most striking was by a fellow aphantasic who also has spatial imagery but no object imagery, and whose experiences in many respects appear to be very similar to mine. For one thing, they also learned to tune in to their heartbeat and subtly change it, but contrary to me, they are convinced that the effect is real. This made me curious enough to do a literature search, which lead me to something called “heart rate variability biofeedback training”,57 which has some similarities to what I (and they) was (were) doing a couple of decades ago. (The main differences are the use of technology to monitor heart rate and the involvement of breath as an intermediate.) So it appears that my suspicion that the apparent change of heart rate is mere illusion might have been groundless. Whether doing something like this actually has any significant effects is uncertain, however. A still unpublished paper by Alessandro Sparacio and colleagues reports the results of a meta-analysis about the effects of self-administered mindfulness meditation and heart rate variability biofeedback on stress, but finds no significant results “due to the low quality of the literature, high heterogeneity, high risk of bias”, and other problems.58
Triggered by some of the papers that were suggested to me, I spend a day searching relevant literature and reading, mostly about empirical research into the effects of meditation. Above I wrote that “a meta-analysis is needed to test whether prior beliefs of the researchers significantly influence research results, but as far as I know, this has not been done yet.” I still don’t know whether this has been done, but there are very, very many meta-analyses of the effects of meditation, so it may very well be the case that I just haven’t been able to find one. I stumbled upon something very similar, however.
One of the more recent meditation-related research themes has to do with the effects of meditation on prosociality (i.e. the extent to which one is concerned with the welfare of others) and its opposite. A few meta-analyses have been published already, and these typically report very small, but statistically significant effects. One meta-analysis stands out, however. Ute Kreplin, Miguel Farias, and Inti Brazil reported that whether a study finds prosocial effects of meditation depends among others on the involvement of the meditation teacher.59 If the teacher – who obviously is most convinced of positive effects and has most to gain by confirming such positive effects – is part of the research team, then a positive effect is found. This seems clear evidence for the confirmation bias I’m worried about, but then again, this is just one meta-analysis.
There is much more that is of interest in the literature on this kind of effects of meditation, however. As mentioned, some meta-analyses report a small, but statistically significant effect of meditation (typically mindfulness meditation) on prosocial behavior, levels of compassion, and so forth, but the studies aggregated in these meta-analyses tend to rely on self-reports. That is, they typically ask people whether they have become more compassionate, caring, or prosocial (or something like that). This is obviously problematic as people tend to be fairly ignorant about their own motivations and have clear incentives to present themselves as “better” than they really are. To address this issue, Simon Schindler and Stefan Pfattheicher tested whether meditation actually makes people more prosocial by doing experiments involving donations and other economic behavior. They found that mindfulness meditation does not actually change people’s behavior – meditators are not more prosocial in their actual behavior than others.60 Another recent study that hasn’t been published yet reveals another important piece of the puzzle about meditation and prosociality: it turns out that whether meditation makes one more or less prosocial depends on one’s wider goals and attitudes. Particularly, the authors found that “for people who tend to view themselves as more independent, mindfulness actually decreased prosocial behavior”.61
Closely related to this decrease in prosociality is an increase in narcissism (which is a widespread aspect of modern culture) and self-centeredness.62 In a paper reporting experimental findings on this latter relation, Roos Vonk and Anouk Visser quote the (rather controversial) Tibetan meditation master Chögyam Trungpa:
There are numerous sidetracks which lead to a distorted, ego-centered version of spirituality; we can deceive ourselves into thinking we are developing spiritually when instead we are strengthening our egocentricity through spiritual techniques. This fundamental distortion may be referred to as spiritual materialism.63
Chögyam’s main point is that “Ego is able to convert everything to its own use, even spirituality”.64 Basicaly, what Roos and Visser do in their paper, is put Chögyam’s claim to the test.65 What they found is that “spiritual” practice, such as meditation, may lead to increases in narcissism, self-absorption, and sense of superiority – that is, to a “distorted, ego-centered version of spirituality” indeed.
Second Addendum (December 6th)
Since posting the previous addendum I got some further feedback from aphantasics, but also from some experienced meditators including a Buddhist monk. This didn’t result in any new insights, however. A common theme in much of the response I got is that aphantasia shouldn’t be an obstacle because imagery is non-essential.75 Indeed, many standard descriptions of, and instructions for meditation lean heavily on imagery, but that’s just because most people (can) experience imagery, so it is an obvious tool. It’s just a tool, however, and there’s no reason why imagery would be necessary. This kind of response isn’t very helpful, because it completely ignores the fact that the fifth dhyāna/jhāna is pretty much defined by mental imagery. Hence, the inability to experience mental imagery makes it very difficult to distinguish some dhyānas. Now, it can of course be argued that trying to distinguish those isn’t that important anyway, but such an argument would only raise the question why those distinctions are made in the first place if they don’t really matter.
Something else that was pointed out to me in discussions about this topic with more experienced meditators is that many Western meditators don’t seem to progress beyond the first or second dhyāna.76 This was also confirmed by what I read in a (non-randomly selected and thus not representative) selection of forum threads (etc.) in which people discussed their own experiences. If what I experienced in meditation wasn’t some kind of deception (i.e. if my experiences that best fit the descriptions of the 5th and 7th dhyānas were indeed experiences of those dhyānas, and so forth) then this raises a question: Why does it seem that I “progressed” through the stages with relative ease (i.e. it only took a few years of practice)? Why didn’t I get “stuck” (as much)? (I’m assuming here that the difference cannot be attributed to a difference of effort, but there really is no reliable data to compare the extent of my effort with others. On a side note, I’m not at all happy with the term “progress” here, as it seems to imply that I somehow “achieved” more than those who “progressed” less, but I don’t think that is the case. Or at least, I don’t feel like I achieved anything or made any “progress” in some relevant sense.77)
supplemental note (Feb. 28, 2022)
As mentioned in a comment with the same date at the end of the first “personal reflections” section, I now think that the explanation that all my meditative experiences where illusions (and/or misinterpretations of flawed memory) is at least as likely as the explanation given here.
An answer to that question – suggested to me by one of the experienced meditators I discussed this with – has to do with aims and expectations. Apparently, many Western meditators engage in meditation to relieve stress, to feel better/happier, or for related reasons. With that kind of aim and expectation, the (often intensely) pleasurable sensations of the second dhyāna may seem to deliver all that one was seeking, and because of that, the meditator gets attached to those sensations, which blocks further “progress”. My main aim was satisfying my curiosity, however. I didn’t expect or aim for particular states, but just wanted to know what kinds of experiences one could (typically) have during meditation. For that reason, I had little reason to get attached to any of these experiences either. In the contrary, with every new experience I just thought “so there’s that; what else is there?” or something like that. Or in other words, I wasn’t in any way personally invested in meditation and meditative experiences and basically approached the whole “thing” with a kind of academic curiosity and detachment. That detachment may have prevented obstructive attachment. I must emphasize, however, that this is a speculative explanation and that there may be other explanations.
A central theme in the response from meditators is the heavy focus on tranquility meditation (most of them completely ignored everything else I wrote about), which is much in line with mainstream Western Buddhism and related currents like Modernist Zen or Modernist Theravāda Buddhism. The aforementioned monk urged me to start meditating “again” (ignoring my other, continuing “meditation” practice), and so did several others, but it seems to me that that advice completely misses one key point.78 This doubtlessly well-meant advice usually neglects to tell me why I should meditate, or if it does so, it only mentions supposed “benefits” that I’m not interested in. I see no great value in peace of mind, “enlightenment” (or “awakening” – in fact, I’m highly skeptical that there even is such a state), or even happiness. There’s an underlying assumption that I do and/or should care about these “things”, but I find that a problematic assumption.
Let’s separate the “do” from the “should” here. To assume that I should care (primarily) about my own peace of mind etcetera is to assume an essentially selfish ultimate goal, which I find problematic on moral grounds as I have explained before. So, I reject the notion that I should care about these things. Now, with regards to the “do” – of course, there are things I value, such as knowledge and not being in severe pain, but one of the main effects of my second kind of “meditation” practice discussed above (and this is its supposed effect!) is that I care a whole lot less about the typical (selfish) goals of tranquility meditation (and its modernist offshoots).79 The more I “meditate” on suffering and death, the less I care about my peace of mind etcetera (and the I more care about the well-being of others, which may involve their peace of mind if that is what they need most).80 So, again, I don’t care about peace of mind etcetera and I don’t think I should care about those either. Furthermore, neither should you. The world would be a much better place if more of us would care more about each other’s well-being81 and less about our own “peace of mind”.
There’s a passage in the proclamation read in the founding meeting of the New Buddhist Youth League (新興仏教青年同盟, Japan, 1931) that perfectly illustrates my problem with the excessive focus on tranquility meditation and its offshoots in Buddhist Modernism:
This is an era of suffering. Fellow men desire love and trust, but are forced to engage in conflict, while the general public wishes for bread, but is only fed oppression. Either if one [tries to] escape or [engages in] conflict, the present world is fluctuating between chaos and distress. In such an age, what are Buddhists aware of, and what are they contributing to society? Intoxicated by [their own] cheap peace of mind, most Buddhists do not see a problem.82
I don’t want to be “intoxicated”. I don’t care about peace of mind. In fact, I find the idea of achieving peace of mind in this world of suffering revolting. How can one be aware of the massive suffering in this world and achieve peace of mind? How can one even desire that? To me, that seems hideously callous, if not psychopathic. If that is what tranquility meditation has to offer, I don’t want it. I don’t need peace of mind, or “enlightenment”, or even happiness. I need an end to the massive suffering of countless others. I need an end to war and hunger, to climate change and capitalism, to pollution and oppression, and so forth.
It seems to me that many of those who emphasize the personal benefits of meditation are awfully close to what Chögyam Trungpa called “spiritual materialism” (see previous addendum) or at the very least have forgotten (or closed their eyes to) what really matters: suffering. What is needed in this “era of suffering”,83 in this age of self-obsessed narcissism (or cultural psychopathy),84 is not some kind of acquired ability to cope with stress and to achieve peace of mind, but a frequent reminder of the nature and ubiquity of suffering, leading to a genuine desire to alleviate some of that suffering. People – and especially “Buddhists” – should open their minds to the suffering of others; not close their minds and look away (or look inside).
But that’s “just my opinion”. Obviously, I have more affinity with the kind of “radicalBuddhism” advocated by Seno’o Girō 妹尾義郎 and others than with more typical Buddhist Modernism. As Śāntideva already pointed out, if Buddhism is supposed to address the problem of suffering, then it should address all suffering. For him, the supreme goal of meditation was “the exchange of self and other”: “in order to allay my own suffering and to allay the suffering of others, I devote myself to others and accept them as myself”.85 In far as anything I do can be called “meditation”, that is – and should be – its goal, regardless of whether it can ever be fully achieved.
If you found this article and/or other articles in this blog useful or valuable, please consider making a small financial contribution to support this blog, 𝐹=𝑚𝑎, and its author. You can find 𝐹=𝑚𝑎’s Patreon pagehere.
Notes
Or certain versions or stages thereof, at least.
Adam Zeman, Michaela Dewar, & Sergio Della Sala (2015), “Lives without Imagery – Congenital Aphantasia”, Cortex 73: 378-380.
There’s a recent Youtube video by Joe Scott explaining aphantasia, which is worth watching if you have never heard of the phenomenon before.
Lajos Brons (2019), “Aphantasia, SDAM, and episodic memory”, Annals of the Japan Association for Philosophy of Science 28: 9-32.
Bill Faw (2009), “Conflicting Intuitions May be Based on Differing Abilities: Evidence from Mental Imaging Research”, Journal of Consciousness Studies 16.4: 45-68, p. 45. — Notice that Faw uses the term “non-imager”, because “aphantasic” had not been coined yet.
Eric Schwitzgebel (2008), “The Unreliability of Naive Introspection”, Philosophical Review 117.2: 245-73. Peter Carruthers (2011), The Opacity of Mind (Oxford: Oxford University Press). Schwitzgebel (2012), “Introspection, What?”, in: D. Smithies & D. Stoljar (eds.), Introspection and Consciousness (Oxford: Oxford University Press): 29-47.
Or at least not from a more or less academic perspective. I have seen some forum posts etcetera about the topic, but those tended to be rather practice-oriented and didn’t answer any of my more theoretical questions.
I asked around on the internet a bit, but that didn’t result in any new insights, unfortunately.
Steven Collins (1990), “On the Very Idea of the Pāli Canon”, reprinted in: Paul Williams (ed.) (2005), Buddhism: Critical Concepts in Religious Studies, Volume I: Buddhist Origins and the Early History of Buddhism in South and Southeast Asia (London: Routledge): 72-95.
Tilman Vetter (1988), The Ideas and Meditative Practices of Early Buddhism (Leiden: Brill).
Johannes Bronkhorst (2009), Buddhist Teaching in India (Boston: Wisdom Publications).
See especially: Lars Fogelin (2015), An Archaeological History of Indian Buddhism (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Donald Lopez jr. (2012), The Scientific Buddha: His Short and Happy Life (New Haven: Yale University Press), p. 82.
Lopez, The Scientific Buddha. Robert Sharf (1995), “Buddhist Modernism and the Rhetoric of Meditative Experience”, Numen 42: 228-83.
For a detailed account, see: Sharf, “Buddhist Modernism and the Rhetoric of Meditative Experience”.
Unless you’d define “Buddhist” in such a way that the vast majority of Buddhists alive today and throughout history suddenly aren’t Buddhist anymore, but that’s so obviously absurd that such Orientalist cultural appropriation doesn’t need to be taken seriously.
Lopez, The Scientific Buddha, p. 83.
Note that the topic here is the category of insight meditation in general, and not the specific interpretation of insight meditation popularized by the aforementioned Vipassanā movement. (See also below.)
The term was also used to refer to a kind of single-minded attention that will ensure that one remembers what one is paying attention to/is mindful of. Hence, monks were instructed to mindfully listen to sermons, so they would remember them.
See also the first addendum below.
Nevertheless, what might be interesting is to investigate whether insight meditation about no-self and impermanence leads to substantially different ways of thinking reflected in differences in brain architecture. Such research would require a rather big sample size (i.e. a large number of test subjects), however, to discern statistically significant effects and exclude other explanations. fMRI scans of just a handful of experienced insight meditators are pretty much useless. Depending on the subtlety of the effects, we might need hundreds to prove that there is a significant effect indeed.
Roderick Bucknell (1993), “Reinterpreting the Jhānas”, Journal of the International Association of Buddhist Studies 16.2: 375-409.
To make things even more confusing, sometimes a ninth dhyāna is mentioned as well. This is a supposed stage that follows the eight dhyāna.
See also: Walpola Rahula (1974), What the Buddha Taught (New York: Grove Press), pp. 38-9 & 68.
Palombo, Daniela J., Claude Alain, Hedvig Söderlund, Wayne Khuu, & Brian Levine (2015). “Severely Deficient Autobiographical Memory (SDAM) in Healthy Adults: A New Mnemonic Syndrome”, Neuropsychologia 72: 105-118.
What I mean here with “getting into the groove” is more like becoming the rhythm, than like focusing one’s attention on something outside one’s (sense of) self.
Some descriptions of the third and fourth dhyānas also involve mental imagery. I’ve read somewhere that in the third dhyāna something is supposed to happen in the margins of one’s mental imagery, for example. As an aphantasic, I don’t know what to make of such imagery-heavy descriptions, which is one reason – for me, at least – to prefer Bucknell’s account mentioned above.
In the late summer of 2013.
I know that it lasted for about 15 minutes because my wife told me that approximately 20 minutes had passed, and I spent a few minutes in preparation.
One rationalization of the sudden indifference after experiencing nothingness is the realization that that experience cannot result in insight in the nature of reality, no-self, and so forth, and I’m much more interested in that kind of insight than in “calming”. But that is a later rationalization, and is certainly not something that went through my mind at the time.
But I feel no aversion either. As mentioned, I’m indifferent.
In the first phase of a migraine, my field of vision gets covered with brightly colored geometrical patterns and blurry areas, which all continuously tremble or vibrate. If it’s particularly bad, I need to somehow distract myself from that, because otherwise I will start throwing up. (This initial phase of a migraine is far worse than the headache that follows it, by the way.) The easiest way to distract me from these symptoms is by forcefully focusing my attention on something else, like my breath or a body part. It doesn’t even matter much whether I can keep my attention focused, as long as I can refocus my attention every time my thoughts distract me. And actually, getting distracted by thoughts works just as well.
Palombo, Daniela J., Claude Alain, Hedvig Söderlund, Wayne Khuu, & Brian Levine (2015). “Severely Deficient Autobiographical Memory (SDAM) in Healthy Adults: A New Mnemonic Syndrome”, Neuropsychologia 72: 105-118.
What I mean here with “getting into the groove” is more like becoming the rhythm, than like focusing one’s attention on something outside one’s (sense of) self.
Some descriptions of the third and fourth dhyānas also involve mental imagery. I’ve read somewhere that in the third dhyāna something is supposed to happen in the margins of one’s mental imagery, for example. As an aphantasic, I don’t know what to make of such imagery-heavy descriptions, which is one reason – for me, at least – to prefer Bucknell’s account mentioned above.
In the late summer of 2013.
I know that it lasted for about 15 minutes because my wife told me that approximately 20 minutes had passed, and I spent a few minutes in preparation.
One rationalization of the sudden indifference after experiencing nothingness is the realization that that experience cannot result in insight in the nature of reality, no-self, and so forth, and I’m much more interested in that kind of insight than in “calming”. But that is a later rationalization, and is certainly not something that went through my mind at the time.
But I feel no aversion either. As mentioned, I’m indifferent.
In the first phase of a migraine, my field of vision gets covered with brightly colored geometrical patterns and blurry areas, which all continuously tremble or vibrate. If it’s particularly bad, I need to somehow distract myself from that, because otherwise I will start throwing up. (This initial phase of a migraine is far worse than the headache that follows it, by the way.) The easiest way to distract me from these symptoms is by forcefully focusing my attention on something else, like my breath or a body part. It doesn’t even matter much whether I can keep my attention focused, as long as I can refocus my attention every time my thoughts distract me. And actually, getting distracted by thoughts works just as well.
Donald Lopez jr. (2008), Buddhism & Science: A Guide for the Perplexed (Chicago: University of Chicago Press), pp. 197-207.
Sharf, “Buddhist Modernism and the Rhetoric of Meditative Experience”.
Lajos Brons (2016), “Facing Death from a Safe Distance: Saṃvega and Moral Psychology”, Journal of Buddhist Ethics 23: 83-128. — There’s an amusing parallel between this paper and my aforementioned paper on aphantasia, by the way. Other aphantasics emailed me about that paper and some of them wrote that I’m obviously not aphantasic myself because of how I described aphantasia. More recently someone read my saṃvega paper and replied that I have obviously never meditated myself because of how I described meditation.
James Baillie (2013), “The Expectation of Nothingness”, Philosophical Studies 166.S: S185-S203, p. S189.
Śāntideva (8th ct/1995), The Bodhicaryāvatāra, Translated by Kate Crosby and Andrew Skilton (Oxford: Oxford University Press), §6:21.
Brons, “Facing Death from a Safe Distance”.
And it makes no sense to say that mettā only applies to concern for the well-being of non-suffering beings.
Which contrasts it with empathy. Empathy is always aimed at a specific other, while mettā is impartial compassion for everyone.
Śāntideva, The Bodhicaryāvatāra, 8:135-7.
8:107.
Thich Nhat Hanh (1987), Being Peace (Berkeley: Parallax).
This practice doesn’t look much like stereotypical “meditation” either, by the way. It requires no particular posture or preparation, for example.
Thich Nhat Hanh (1987), Being Peace (Berkeley: Parallax).
This practice doesn’t look much like stereotypical “meditation” either, by the way. It requires no particular posture or preparation, for example.
The first character 佛 means “Buddha”, but in a general sense. In compounds, it refers to either Buddhism or to awakening (bodhi), but (almost?) never to the historical Buddha.
V. Goessl, J. Curtiss, & S. Hofmann (2017), “The effect of heart rate variability biofeedback training on stress and anxiety: A meta-analysis” Psychological Medicine 47.15: 2578-86.
Alessandro Sparacio et al., “Stress regulation via self-administered mindfulness and biofeedback interventions in adults: A pre-registered meta-analysis”, https://psyarxiv.com/zpw28/
Ute Kreplin, Miguel Farias, and Inti Brazil (2018), “The limited prosocial effects of meditation: A systematic review and meta-analysis” Nature Scientific Reports 8:2403,
Simon Schindler & Stefan Pfattheicher (2021), “When it really counts: Investigating the relation between trait mindfulness and actual prosocial behavior”, Current Psychology.
Michael Poulin, Lauren Ministero, Shira Gabriel, Carrie Morrison, (&) Esha Naidu, “Minding your own business? Mindfulness decreases prosocial behavior for those with independent self-construals”, https://psyarxiv.com/xhyua — Emphasis added.
Jean M. Twenge and W. Keith Campbell (2009), The Narcissism Epidemic: Living in the Age of Entitlement (New York: Atria). — I prefer to call this phenomenon “cultural psychopathy” rather than “narcissism”, by the way. See: Lajos Brons (2017), The Hegemony of Psychopathy (Santa Barbara: Brainstorm).
Chögyam Trungpa (1973), Cutting Through Spiritual Materialism (Boston: Shambala, 2002), p. 3. — Vonk & Visser only quote the first sentence, but also adopt the term “spiritual materialism”.
Ibidem, p. 7.
Roos Vonk & Anouk Visser (2020), “An exploration of spiritual superiority: The paradox of self-enhancement”, European Journal of Social Psychology 51.1: 152-65.
V. Goessl, J. Curtiss, & S. Hofmann (2017), “The effect of heart rate variability biofeedback training on stress and anxiety: A meta-analysis” Psychological Medicine 47.15: 2578-86.
Alessandro Sparacio et al., “Stress regulation via self-administered mindfulness and biofeedback interventions in adults: A pre-registered meta-analysis”, https://psyarxiv.com/zpw28/
Ute Kreplin, Miguel Farias, and Inti Brazil (2018), “The limited prosocial effects of meditation: A systematic review and meta-analysis” Nature Scientific Reports 8:2403,
Simon Schindler & Stefan Pfattheicher (2021), “When it really counts: Investigating the relation between trait mindfulness and actual prosocial behavior”, Current Psychology.
Michael Poulin, Lauren Ministero, Shira Gabriel, Carrie Morrison, (&) Esha Naidu, “Minding your own business? Mindfulness decreases prosocial behavior for those with independent self-construals”, https://psyarxiv.com/xhyua — Emphasis added.
Jean M. Twenge and W. Keith Campbell (2009), The Narcissism Epidemic: Living in the Age of Entitlement (New York: Atria). — I prefer to call this phenomenon “cultural psychopathy” rather than “narcissism”, by the way. See: Lajos Brons (2017), The Hegemony of Psychopathy (Santa Barbara: Brainstorm).
Chögyam Trungpa (1973), Cutting Through Spiritual Materialism (Boston: Shambala, 2002), p. 3. — Vonk & Visser only quote the first sentence, but also adopt the term “spiritual materialism”.
Ibidem, p. 7.
Roos Vonk & Anouk Visser (2020), “An exploration of spiritual superiority: The paradox of self-enhancement”, European Journal of Social Psychology 51.1: 152-65.
In fact, many other “details” appear to be considered inessential as well. A typical response I got was along the lines “if it works for you then it is right”.
This doesn’t imply that many non-Western meditators do progress beyond that point. The latter didn’t come up in discussion and I didn’t research it either.
This is also one of the main reasons why I really don’t like talking or writing about this topic (and why I haven’t done so until now). It can too easily sound like a very distasteful kind of boasting about one’s own “spiritual achievements” (even despite the fact that I’m not spiritual and don’t feel like I have “achieved” anything).
Historically, it is also very peculiar for a monk to advise a layman to meditate, considering that even most monks don’t meditate. The focus on meditation in Buddhism is of 20th century origin (see above), and throughout history, Buddhist laymen almost never meditated.
Note that this doesn’t explain my initial indifference after experiencing nothingness, as that predates my experience with saṃvega-based meditation.
An interesting question is how much of this change is due to this “meditation” practice and how much is due to fatherhood, especially in a primary care-giver role, given that scientific research has shown that being a primary care-giver (or giving care in general) leads to changes in the brain that actually make one more caring.
As long as that is an impartial care and isn’t directed at specific, selected, preferred others.
My translation. 「現代は苦悩する。同胞は信愛を欲して闘争を余儀なくされ、大衆はパンを求めて弾圧を食べらわされる。逃避か闘争か、今や世はあげて混沌と窮迫とに彷徨する。 かかる現代、仏教徒は何を認識し、何を社会に寄与しつつあるか。安価な安心に陶酔しておる多数仏教徒は問題とすまい。」 新興仏教青年同盟 (New Buddhist Youth League) (1931), 『宣言』 (Proclamation), reprinted in: 稲垣真美 (1974), 『仏陀を背負いて街頭へ—妹尾義郎と新興仏教青年同盟』 (岩波新書): 3-6, p. 3.
See the block quote above.
Twenge & Campbell, The Narcissism Epidemic. Brons, The Hegemony of Psychopathy.
More fully quoted above.
In fact, many other “details” appear to be considered inessential as well. A typical response I got was along the lines “if it works for you then it is right”.
This doesn’t imply that many non-Western meditators do progress beyond that point. The latter didn’t come up in discussion and I didn’t research it either.
This is also one of the main reasons why I really don’t like talking or writing about this topic (and why I haven’t done so until now). It can too easily sound like a very distasteful kind of boasting about one’s own “spiritual achievements” (even despite the fact that I’m not spiritual and don’t feel like I have “achieved” anything).
Historically, it is also very peculiar for a monk to advise a layman to meditate, considering that even most monks don’t meditate. The focus on meditation in Buddhism is of 20th century origin (see above), and throughout history, Buddhist laymen almost never meditated.
Note that this doesn’t explain my initial indifference after experiencing nothingness, as that predates my experience with saṃvega-based meditation.
An interesting question is how much of this change is due to this “meditation” practice and how much is due to fatherhood, especially in a primary care-giver role, given that scientific research has shown that being a primary care-giver (or giving care in general) leads to changes in the brain that actually make one more caring.
As long as that is an impartial care and isn’t directed at specific, selected, preferred others.
My translation. 「現代は苦悩する。同胞は信愛を欲して闘争を余儀なくされ、大衆はパンを求めて弾圧を食べらわされる。逃避か闘争か、今や世はあげて混沌と窮迫とに彷徨する。 かかる現代、仏教徒は何を認識し、何を社会に寄与しつつあるか。安価な安心に陶酔しておる多数仏教徒は問題とすまい。」 新興仏教青年同盟 (New Buddhist Youth League) (1931), 『宣言』 (Proclamation), reprinted in: 稲垣真美 (1974), 『仏陀を背負いて街頭へ—妹尾義郎と新興仏教青年同盟』 (岩波新書): 3-6, p. 3.
See the block quote above.
Twenge & Campbell, The Narcissism Epidemic. Brons, The Hegemony of Psychopathy.